One of the key ideas in modern
sociology is social construction. This indicates that a person’s perception of
reality is to a large extent constructed by the society in which that person
lives. For example, if I perceive the
people downstream to be untrustworthy, that may have nothing to do with how
they actually behave; it is all to do with how my own people teach me to look
at them.
When the biopolitical sciences
emerged in the 1970’s, they seemed to present a stark alternative to that
idea. The way we perceive reality is
largely conditioned by natural selection.
My genes determine how I will react in any context because those genes
were selected for: the coded traits are the ones that got their genes into my
mother and father.
It has been clear for some time
that this dichotomy was an obstacle to the truth. In even the simplest creatures, genes code
for a range of responses to the environment.
Even a tree can learn when to shed its leaves by responding to the
coming of winter.
As I
noted in a previous post, cleaner fish are more likely to be honest
when they are observed by a number of potential client fish. The client fish want a cleaner who will
confine his appetites to the ectoparasites, thus performing the cleaning. Doing just that when potential clients are
looking on is a good business model.
That is social construction. Their perception of the situation is
constructed, to some degree, by the social situation. In this case, the social situation includes
non-conspecific organisms.
I have been reading recently
about Norwegian rats (Rattus norvegicus).
These rats are very good at gauging reciprocal exchanges. They share food, but are more likely to share
with an individual depending on the quality of food that the latter shared in
the past. If he/she gave me good stuff
last time, he/she is worth repaying.
That is direct reciprocity. You
return a favor based on your record of past exchanges. Another kind of relationship is indirect reciprocity. If C sees A doing a favor for B, C is more
likely to do a favor for A. A is a
standup guy. We see something of this in
the cleaner fish example. Strong reciprocity adds an element of
punishment. If A doesn’t play by the
rules, I won’t play with A. That seems
to be at work in vampire bats who refuse to share with a stingy roost mate.
But there is another kind of
reciprocity that is very interesting because it makes fewer demands on the
cognitive development of the participants.
In all of the above cases, you have to have a brain sophisticated enough
to keep track of individual encounters.
It is difficult to see how that develops unless there is already a lot
of cooperation going on.
Perhaps an easier route to
reciprocity is just to measure the general level of cooperation in the group
that you happen to be in at the time. The
more cooperative partners you encounter, the more you cooperate and vice
versa.
That is generalized reciprocity, and it has been observed in rats. In a piece by Claudia Rutte and Michael
Taborsky, female rats where more likely to cooperate if they had received help
in the past, regardless of the identity of the potential partner. Once they sensed they were in a good
neighborhood, they became good neighbors.
Of course, if the neighborhood is bad…
It struck me tonight as I was
reading a very interesting book‑Other
Minds: The Octopus, The Sea, and the Deep Origins of Consciousness, by
Peter Godfrey-Smith‑that this an example of the primary social mechanism in
living organisms. Godfrey-Smith offers
the example of a glowing squid.
Luminescence provides a big advantage to this creature. It allows it to blend in with the moonlit
background (upground), so that its shadow doesn’t warn its prey. How does it manage this trick?
The squid provides a home to
bacteria that can luminesce. That,
however, is expensive, biologically speaking.
There is no point in bothering if there aren’t enough of your clones
around to produce a descent bit of light.
The bacteria rely on their ability to both sense and produce an “inducer”
molecule. That allows each little
bacterium to tell how many of his fellows are around. This is called quorum sensing, a remarkably political term. When the inducer molecules reach a certain
density in the local environment (the Hawaiian squid) the bacteria turn on
their lights.
The rats are much more advanced
creatures than the bacteria but the mechanism seems only a little more
advanced. What the rat needs to know isn’t
how big the local population is but what is its moral character? Its own moral behavior (cooperate or not) is
determined by its finding. This doesn’t
require anything more sophisticated than the ability to sample and effectively
draw conclusions.
The social construction of
individual character is pervasive among living organisms. It is clearly developed in a high degree in
human organisms. The sociologists were
right to put a strong emphasis on social construction. They were wrong to suppose that this
mechanism somehow freed human beings from biological causation or that it could
be properly understood without biology.
I fear that it will take a change
of guard across the social sciences for the full integration of biology and the
former to more fully and fruitfully integrate.
The general fields of sociology and political science are still very
resistant to this type of research. This
may amount to a tragedy. Higher
education is changing in ways that are not favorable to either the social
sciences or the humanities. It would be
a tragedy indeed if the former would diminished just when the greatest
potential for discover and application was at hand.
No comments:
Post a Comment